Research Article
BibTex RIS Cite

ANASINIFINDAN DÖRDÜNCÜ SINIFA KADAR OLAN ÇOCUKLAR İÇİN GELİŞTİRİLMİŞ HIZLI İSİMLENDİRME TESTİ’NİN GEÇERLİK VE GÜVENİRLİĞİ

Year 2022, Volume: 12 Issue: 1, 176 - 192, 26.01.2022
https://doi.org/10.24315/tred.883688

Abstract

Bellek tarafından ulaşılabilen sesbilgisinin hızına ilişkin bir gösterge olan hızlı isimlendirme becerileri değerlendirildiğinde okuma güçlüğü yaşayan çocukların ayırt edilmesine ve erken dönemde belirlenmesine yönelik güçlü ve güvenilir bilgiler sağlamaktadır. Bu çalışmada anasınıfından dördüncü sınıfa kadar olan sınıf düzeylerindeki çocuklar (60-125 ay arası) için geliştirilmiş, nesne, renk, harf ve rakam isimlendirmeye dayalı dört alt testten oluşan Hızlı İsimlendirme Testinin (HİT) geçerlik ve güvenirliğin incelenmesi ve kesme aralıkları ile değerlendirme ölçütlerinin belirlenmesi amaçlanmıştır. Bu kapsamda 60-125 ay arasındaki 860 çocuk ile çalışmalar yürütülmüştür. Analizler sonucunda HİT’in uzman görüşleri çerçevesinde kapsam geçerliğine sahip olduğu, açımlayıcı ve doğrulaycı faktör analizleri sonucunda ise yüksek bir varyans oranı ve model-veri uyumu ile yapı geçerliğini sağladığı belirlenmiştir. Sonuçlar ayrıca HİT’in sınıf düzeyleri ortalamaları arasındaki farka dayalı ayırıcılık geçerliğinin ve yüksek bir test tekrar test güvenirliğinin olduğunu göstermiştir. Çalışmada son olarak HİT’in sınıf düzeyleri temelinde ve 60-125 ay arasında 6 aylık yaş dilimlerinde değerlendirmeye esas kesme aralıkları belirlenmiştir.

Supporting Institution

TÜBİTAK

Project Number

215K027

References

  • Araujo, S., Reis, A., Petersson, K. M. & Faísca, L. (2015). Rapid automatized naming and reading performance: A meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(3), 868-883. doi:10.1037/edu0000006
  • Babayiğit, S. ve Stainthorp, R. (2011). Modeling the relationships between cognitive linguistic skills and literacy skills: New insights from a transparent orthography. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103(1), 169-189.
  • Badian, N. A. (1994). Preschool prediction: Orthographic and phonological skills, and reading. Annals of Dyslexia, 44, 3-25. Bakır ve Babür (2018). Hızlı Otomatik İsimlendirme Testi’nin Türkçeye uyarlanması. Boğaziçi Üniversitesi Eğitim Dergisi, 35(2), 35-50.
  • Bexkens, A., Wildenberg, W. P. & Tijms, J. (2014). Rapid automatized naming in children with dyslexia: Is inhibitory control involved? Dyslexia, 21, 212–234.
  • Bishop, A. G. & League, M. B. (2006). Identifying a multivariate screening model to predict reading difficulties at the onset of kindergarten: A longitudinal analysis. Learning Disability Quarterly, 29, 235–252.
  • Boets, B., De Smedt, B., Cleuren, L., Vandewalle, E., Wouters, J. & Ghesquiere, P. (2010). Towards a further characterization of phonological and literacy problems in Dutch-speaking children with dyslexia. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28, 5–31.
  • Bowey, J. A. (2005). Predicting individual differences in learning to read. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 155–172). Oxford: Blackwell.
  • Bowey, J. A., McGuigan, M. & Ruschena, A. (2005). On the association between serial naming speed for letters and digits and word‐reading skill: Towards a developmental account. Journal of Research in Reading, 28(4), 400-422.
  • Çakıroğlu, O. & Melekoğlu, M. A. (2014). Statistical trends and developments within inclusive education in Turkey. International Journal of Inclusive Education, 18(8), 798-808. doi:10.1080/13603116.2013.836573
  • Catts, H. W., Fey, M. E., Zhang, X. & Tomblin, J. B. (1999). Language basis of reading and reading disabilities: Evidence from a longitudinal investigation. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3, 331–361.
  • Catts, H. W., Gillispie, M., Leonard, L. B., Kail, R. V. & Miller, C. A. (2002). The role of speed of processing, rapid naming, and phonological awareness in reading achievement. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 509–524.
  • Christo, C. & Davis, J. (2008). Rapid naming and phonological processing as predictors of reading and spelling. The California School Psychologist, 13(1), 7-18.
  • Clarke, P., Hulme, C. & Snowling, M. (2005). Individual differences in RAN and reading: A response timing analysis. Journal of Research in Reading, 28, 73–86.
  • Compton, D. L., Davis, C. J., DeFries, J. C., Gayan, J. & Olson, R. K. (2001). Genetic and environmental influences on reading and RAN: An overview of results from the Colorado twin study. In M. Wolf (Ed.), Time, Fluency, and Developmental Dyslexia (pp. 277–303). Baltimore, MD: York Press.
  • Compton, D. L., Olson, R. K., DeFries, J. C. & Pennington, B. F. (2002). Comparing the relationships among two different versions of alphanumeric rapid automatized naming and word level reading skills. Scientific Studies of Reading, 6, 343–368.
  • Compton, D. (2003). Modeling the relationship between growth in rapid naming speed and growth in decoding skill in first-grade children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 225–239.
  • De Jong, P. F. & Vrielink, L. O. (2004). Rapid automatic naming: Easy to measure, hard to improve (quickly). Annals of Dyslexia, 54, 65–88.
  • De Jong, P. F. & Van Der Leij, A. (2003). Developmental changes in the manifestation of a phonological deficit in dyslexic children learning to read a regular orthography. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 22–40.
  • Demirtaş, Ç. P. & Ergül, D. (2020). Düşük okuma başarısı gösteren çocuklarda okuma, sesbilgisel farkındalık, hızlı isimlendirme ve çalışma belleği becerilerinin incelenmesi. Ankara Üniversitesi Eğitim Bilimleri Fakültesi Dergisi, 53(1), 209-240. doi:10.30964/auebfd.479111
  • Denckla, M. B. & Rudel, R. G. (1974). Rapid “automatized” naming of pictured objects, colors, letters, and numbers by normal children. Cortex, 10, 186-202.
  • Denckla, M. B. & Rudel, R. G. (1976). Rapid “automatized” naming (R.A.N.): Dyslexia differentiated from other learning disabilities. Neuropsychologia, 14, 471-479.
  • Elwer, A., Keenan, J. M., Olson, R. K., Byrne, B. & Samuelsson, S. (2013). Longitudinal stability and predictors of poor oral comprehenders and poor decoders. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115(3), 497-516.
  • Ergül, C. & Demir, E. (2017). SED indeks ebeveyn bilgi formu. Yayımlanmamış proje raporu.
  • Ergül, C., Ökcün-Akçamuş, M. Ç., Akoğlu, G., Bahap Kudret, Z., Kılıç Tülü B., Demir, E. & Okşak F. E. (2020). Türkçe konuşan çocuklarda fonolojik işlemleme becerileri ilk okuma performansını yorduyor mu? Boylamsal sonuçlar. Hacettepe Üniversitesi Eğitim Fakültesi Dergisi, doi:10.16986/HUJE.2020062765
  • Felton, R. H. (1993). Effects of instruction on the decoding skills of children with phonological-processing problems. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 26(9), 583-589.
  • Felton, R. H. & Brown, I. S. (1990). Phonological processes as predictors of specific reading skills in children at risk for reading failure. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2, 212–229.
  • Furnes, B. & Samuelsson, S. (2011). Phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming predicting early development in reading and spelling: Results from a cross-linguistic longitudinal study. Learning and Individual Differences, 21(1), 85–95. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2010.10.005.
  • Georgiou, G. K., Das, J. P. & Hayward, D. V. (2008). Comparing the contribution of two tests of working memory to reading in relation to phonological awareness and rapid naming speed. Journal of Research in Reading, 31(3), 302-318.
  • Georgiou, G. K., Papadopoulos, T. C., Fella, A. & Parrila, R. (2012). Rapid naming speed components and reading development in a consistent orthography. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112, 1–17.
  • Georgiou, G. K., Parrila, R. & Papadopoulos, T. C. (2008). Predictors of word decoding and reading fluency across languages varying in orthographic consistency. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(3), 566-580.
  • Georgiou, G. K., Parrila, R., Cui, Y. & Papadopoulos, T. C. (2013). Why is rapid automatized naming related to reading? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115(1), 218-225.
  • Georgiou, G., Parrila, R., Kirby, R. & Stephenson, K. (2008). Rapid naming components and their relationship with phonological awareness, orthographic knowledge, speed of processing, and different reading outcomes. Scientific Studies of Reading, 12, 325–350.
  • Güzel Özmen, R. (2008). Öğrenme güçlüğü olan öğrenciler. In İ. H. Diken (Ed.), Özel gereksinimi olan öğrenciler ve özel eğitim (pp. 333-367). Ankara, Turkey: Pegem Akademi. doi:10.14527/9786055885267
  • Hammill, D. D. (2004). What we know about correlates of reading. Exceptional children, 70(4), 453-469. doi:10.1177/001440290407000405
  • Heikkila, R., Narhi, V., Aro, M. & Ahonen, T. (2009) Rapid automatized naming and learning disabilities: Does RAN have a specific connection to reading or not? Child Neuropsychology, 15(4), 343-358, doi:10.1080/09297040802537653
  • Karadağ, Ö., Keskin, H. K. & Arı, G. (2019). Mediatory effect of reading skill in the rapid automatized naming/reading comprehension relationship. Education and Science, 44(197), 353-366.
  • Katz, L. J., Brown, F. C., Roth, R. M. &Beers, S. R. (2011). Processing speed and working memory performance in those with both ADHD and a reading disorder compared with those with ADHD alone. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 26, 425–433.
  • Kirby, J. R., Georgiou, K., Martinussen, R. & Parrila, R. (2010). Naming speed and reading: From prediction to instruction. Reading Research Quarterly, 45, 341–362.
  • Kirby, J. R., Parrila, R. & Pfeiffer, S. (2003). Naming speed and phonological awareness as predictors of reading development. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 453–464.
  • Klein, R. M. (2002). Observations on the temporal correlates of reading failure. Reading and Writing, 15(1), 207–231.
  • Koponen, T., Aunola, K., Ahonen, T. & Nurmi, J. E. (2007). Cognitive predictors of single-digit and procedural calculation and their covariation with reading skill. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 97, 220–241.
  • Koponen, T., Salmi, P., Eklund, K. & Aro, T. (2013). Counting and RAN: Predictors of arithmetic calculation and reading fluency. Journal of Educational psychology, 105, 162- 175.
  • Korhonen, T. T. (1995). The persistence of rapid naming problems in children with reading disabilities: A nine-year follow-up. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 28, 232–239.
  • Landerl, K. & Wimmer, H. (2008). Development of word reading fluency and spelling in a consistent orthography: An 8-year follow-up. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 150–161.
  • Landerl, K., Fussenegger, B., Moll, K. & Willburger, E. (2009). Dyslexia and dyscalculia: Two learning disorders with different cognitive profiles. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103, 309–324.
  • Lovett, M. W. (1987). A developmental approach to reading disability: Accuracy and speed criteria of normal and deficient reading skill. Child Development, 58, 234–260.
  • Manis, F. R., Doi, L. M. & Bhadha, B. (2000). Naming speed, phonological awareness, and ortographic knowledge in second graders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33(4), 325–333.
  • Martinussen, R., Grimbos, T. & Ferrari, J. L. (2014). Word-level reading achievement and behavioral inattention: exploring their overlap and relations with naming speed and phonemic awareness in a community sample of children. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 29, 680–690.
  • Melby-Lervag, M., Lyster, S. A. H. & Hulme, C. (2012). Phonological skills and their role in learning to read: a meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 138(2), 322-352. Melekoğlu, M. A., Erden, G. H., & Çakıroğlu, O. (2019). Development of the Oral Reading Skills and Comprehension Test-II (SOBAT®-II) for assessment of Turkish children with specific learning disabilities: Pilot study results. Journal of Educational Issues, 5(2), 135-149 doi:10.5296/jei.v5i2.15379
  • Meyer, M. S., Wood, F. B., Hart, L. A. & Felton, R. H. (1998). Selective predictive value of rapid automatized naming in poor readers. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 31(2), 106–117.
  • NELP. (2008). Developing early literacy: Report of the National Early Literacy Panel.Washington, DC: National Institute for Literacy.
  • Neuhaus, G., Foorman, B. R., Francis, D. J. & Carlson, C. D. (2001). Measures of information processing in rapid automatizednaming (RAN) and their relation to reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 78, 359–373.
  • Norton, E. S. & Wolf, M. (2012). Rapid automatized naming (RAN) and reading fluency: Implications for understanding and treatment of reading disabilities. The Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 427–52. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100431
  • O’Malley, K. J., Francis, D. J., Foorman, B. R., Fletcher, J. M. & Swank, P. R. (2002). Growth in precursor and reading-related skills: Do low-achieving and IQ-discrepant readers develop differently? Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 17, 19–34.
  • Özata, H. & Haznedar, B. (2018). İlköğretim ikinci sınıfta akıcı sözcük okuma ve okuduğunu anlamayı etkileyen faktörler. Boğaziçi Üniversitesi Eğitim Dergisi, 35(2), 1-33.
  • Pennington, B. F., Cardoso-Martins, C., Green, P. A. & Lefly, D. L. (2001). Comparing the phonological and the double deficit hypotheses of developmental dyslexia. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 14, 707–755.
  • Poulsen, M., Juul, H. & Elbro, C. (2015). Multiple mediation analysis of the relationship between rapid naming and reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 38(2), 124-140. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9817.2012.01547.x
  • Powell, D., Stainthorp, R., Stuart, M., Garwood, H. & Quinlan, P. (2007). An experimental comparison between rival theories of rapid automatized naming performance and its relationship to reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 98(1), 46-68.
  • Puolakanaho, A., Ahonen, T., Aro, M., Eklund, K., Leppanen, P., Poikkeus, A. M., et al. (2007). Very early phonological and language skills: Estimating individual risk of reading disability. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 923–931
  • Raschle, N. M., Chang, M. & Gaab, N. (2011). Structural brain alterations associated with dyslexia predate reading onset. NeuroImage 57, 742–749.
  • Reiter, B. A. (2001). Reading disabilities related to word recognition: Underlying deficits and diagnostic approaches. Canadian Journal of School Psychology, 17, 65–84.
  • Schatschneider, C., Carlson, C. D., Francis, D. J., Foorman, B. R. & Fletcher, J. M. (2002). Relationship of rapid automatized naming and phonological awareness in early reading development: Implications for the double deficit hypothesis. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 245–256.
  • Schatschneider, C., Fletcher, J. M., Francis, D. J., Carlson, C. D. & Foorman, B. R. (2004). Kindergarten prediction of reading skills: A longitudinal comparative analysis. Journal of Education & Psychology, 96, 265–282.
  • Seçkin-Yılmaz, Ş. & Büyükçakmak, Y. (2020). Öğrenme güçlüğü olan ve olmayan öğrencilerin sözcük okuma ve ses bilgisel işlemleme becerilerinin incelenmesi. Kalem Eğitim ve İnsan Bilimleri Dergisi, 10(2), 365-391. doi:10.23863/kalem.2020.161
  • Shaul, S. & Nevo, E. (2015). Different speed of processing levels in childhood and their contribution to early literacy and reading abilities. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 32, 193-203.
  • Suk-Han Ho, C. & Ngar-Chilai, D. (1999). Naming-speed deficits and phonological memory deficits in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Learning and Individual Differences, 11(2), 173-186.
  • Temple, C. M. & Sherwood, S. (2002). Representation and retrieval of arithmetic facts: Developmental difficulties. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55, 733–752.
  • Torgesen, J. K. (2004). Avoiding the devastating downward spiral: The evidence that early intervention prevents reading failure. American Educator, 28, 6–19.
  • Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K. & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). Longitudinal studies of phonological processing and reading. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 27(5), 276-286.
  • Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C. A., Burgess, S. & Hecht, S. (1997). Contributions of phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming ability to the growth of word-reading skills in second- to fifth-grade children. Scientific Study of Reading, 1, 161–195.
  • Torppa, M., Lyytinen, P., Erskine, J., Eklund, K. & Lyytinen, H. (2010). Language development, literacy skills, and predictive connections to reading in Finnish children with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43, 308–321.
  • Vaessen, A. & Blomert, L. (2010). Long-term cognitive dynamics of fluent reading development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105, 213–231.
  • Van Bergen, E., de Jong, P. F., Maassen, B. & van der Leij, A. (2014). The effect of parents’ literacy skills and children’s preliteracy skills on the risk of dyslexia. Journal of abnormal child psychology, 42(7), 1187-1200.
  • Van der Sluis, S., De Jong, P. F. & van der Leij, A. (2004). Inhibition and shifting in children with learning deficits in arithmetic and reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87, 239–266.
  • Vellutino, F. R., Scanlon, D. M. & Tanzman, M. S. (1998). The case for early intervention in diagnosing specific reading disability. Journal of School Psychology, 36, 367–397.
  • Vukovic, R. K., Wilson, A. M. & Nash, K. K. (2004). Naming speed deficits in adults with reading disabilities Journal of Learning Disabilities, 37, 440–450.
  • Waber, D. P., Forbes, P. W., Wolff, P. H. & Weiler, M. D. (2004). Neurodevelopmental characteristics of children with learning impairments classified according to the double-deficit hypothesis. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 37, 451–461.
  • Waber, D. P., Wolff, P. H., Forbes, P. W. & Weiler, M. D. (2000). Rapid automatized naming in children referred for evaluation of heterogeneous learning problems: How specific are naming speed deficits to reading disability? Child Neuropsychology, 6, 251–261.
  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K. & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing (CTOPP). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
  • Wagner, R. K. & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101(2), 192-212.
  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K. & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). Development of reading-reiatedphonological processing abilities: New evidence of bidirectional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 30, 73-87.
  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., Hecht, S. A., Barker, T. A., Burgess, S. R. vd. (1997). Changing relations between phonological processing abilities and word-level reading as children develop from beginning to skilled readers: A 5-year longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 33(3), 468–479.
  • Walsh, D., Price, G. & Gillingham, M. (1988). The critical but transitory importance of letter naming. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 108-122.
  • Wolf, M. & Denckla, M. B. (2005). RAN/RAS: Rapid Automatized Naming and Rapid Alternating Stimulus Tests. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
  • Wolf, M. (1997). A provisional integrative account of phonological and naming speed deficits in dyslexia. In B. Blachman (Ed.), Foundation of reading acquisition (pp. 67–92). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
  • Wolf, M. & Bowers, P. (2000). The question of naming-speed deficits in developmental reading disability: An introduction to the Double-Deficit Hypothesis. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 322–324.
  • Wolf, M. & Bowers, P. G. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 415–438.
  • Wolf, M., Bally, H. & Morris, R. (1986). Automaticity, retrieval processes, and reading: A longitudinal study in average and impaired readers. Child Development, 57, 988-1005.
  • Wolf, M., Bowers, P. & Biddle, K. (2000). Naming-speed processes, timing, and reading: A conceptual review. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 387–407.
  • Wolff, U. (2014). RAN as a predictor of reading skills, and vice versa: Results from a randomised reading intervention. Annals of Dyslexia, 64, 151–165. doi:10.1007/s11881-014-0091-6
Year 2022, Volume: 12 Issue: 1, 176 - 192, 26.01.2022
https://doi.org/10.24315/tred.883688

Abstract

Project Number

215K027

References

  • Araujo, S., Reis, A., Petersson, K. M. & Faísca, L. (2015). Rapid automatized naming and reading performance: A meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(3), 868-883. doi:10.1037/edu0000006
  • Babayiğit, S. ve Stainthorp, R. (2011). Modeling the relationships between cognitive linguistic skills and literacy skills: New insights from a transparent orthography. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103(1), 169-189.
  • Badian, N. A. (1994). Preschool prediction: Orthographic and phonological skills, and reading. Annals of Dyslexia, 44, 3-25. Bakır ve Babür (2018). Hızlı Otomatik İsimlendirme Testi’nin Türkçeye uyarlanması. Boğaziçi Üniversitesi Eğitim Dergisi, 35(2), 35-50.
  • Bexkens, A., Wildenberg, W. P. & Tijms, J. (2014). Rapid automatized naming in children with dyslexia: Is inhibitory control involved? Dyslexia, 21, 212–234.
  • Bishop, A. G. & League, M. B. (2006). Identifying a multivariate screening model to predict reading difficulties at the onset of kindergarten: A longitudinal analysis. Learning Disability Quarterly, 29, 235–252.
  • Boets, B., De Smedt, B., Cleuren, L., Vandewalle, E., Wouters, J. & Ghesquiere, P. (2010). Towards a further characterization of phonological and literacy problems in Dutch-speaking children with dyslexia. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28, 5–31.
  • Bowey, J. A. (2005). Predicting individual differences in learning to read. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 155–172). Oxford: Blackwell.
  • Bowey, J. A., McGuigan, M. & Ruschena, A. (2005). On the association between serial naming speed for letters and digits and word‐reading skill: Towards a developmental account. Journal of Research in Reading, 28(4), 400-422.
  • Çakıroğlu, O. & Melekoğlu, M. A. (2014). Statistical trends and developments within inclusive education in Turkey. International Journal of Inclusive Education, 18(8), 798-808. doi:10.1080/13603116.2013.836573
  • Catts, H. W., Fey, M. E., Zhang, X. & Tomblin, J. B. (1999). Language basis of reading and reading disabilities: Evidence from a longitudinal investigation. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3, 331–361.
  • Catts, H. W., Gillispie, M., Leonard, L. B., Kail, R. V. & Miller, C. A. (2002). The role of speed of processing, rapid naming, and phonological awareness in reading achievement. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 509–524.
  • Christo, C. & Davis, J. (2008). Rapid naming and phonological processing as predictors of reading and spelling. The California School Psychologist, 13(1), 7-18.
  • Clarke, P., Hulme, C. & Snowling, M. (2005). Individual differences in RAN and reading: A response timing analysis. Journal of Research in Reading, 28, 73–86.
  • Compton, D. L., Davis, C. J., DeFries, J. C., Gayan, J. & Olson, R. K. (2001). Genetic and environmental influences on reading and RAN: An overview of results from the Colorado twin study. In M. Wolf (Ed.), Time, Fluency, and Developmental Dyslexia (pp. 277–303). Baltimore, MD: York Press.
  • Compton, D. L., Olson, R. K., DeFries, J. C. & Pennington, B. F. (2002). Comparing the relationships among two different versions of alphanumeric rapid automatized naming and word level reading skills. Scientific Studies of Reading, 6, 343–368.
  • Compton, D. (2003). Modeling the relationship between growth in rapid naming speed and growth in decoding skill in first-grade children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 225–239.
  • De Jong, P. F. & Vrielink, L. O. (2004). Rapid automatic naming: Easy to measure, hard to improve (quickly). Annals of Dyslexia, 54, 65–88.
  • De Jong, P. F. & Van Der Leij, A. (2003). Developmental changes in the manifestation of a phonological deficit in dyslexic children learning to read a regular orthography. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 22–40.
  • Demirtaş, Ç. P. & Ergül, D. (2020). Düşük okuma başarısı gösteren çocuklarda okuma, sesbilgisel farkındalık, hızlı isimlendirme ve çalışma belleği becerilerinin incelenmesi. Ankara Üniversitesi Eğitim Bilimleri Fakültesi Dergisi, 53(1), 209-240. doi:10.30964/auebfd.479111
  • Denckla, M. B. & Rudel, R. G. (1974). Rapid “automatized” naming of pictured objects, colors, letters, and numbers by normal children. Cortex, 10, 186-202.
  • Denckla, M. B. & Rudel, R. G. (1976). Rapid “automatized” naming (R.A.N.): Dyslexia differentiated from other learning disabilities. Neuropsychologia, 14, 471-479.
  • Elwer, A., Keenan, J. M., Olson, R. K., Byrne, B. & Samuelsson, S. (2013). Longitudinal stability and predictors of poor oral comprehenders and poor decoders. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115(3), 497-516.
  • Ergül, C. & Demir, E. (2017). SED indeks ebeveyn bilgi formu. Yayımlanmamış proje raporu.
  • Ergül, C., Ökcün-Akçamuş, M. Ç., Akoğlu, G., Bahap Kudret, Z., Kılıç Tülü B., Demir, E. & Okşak F. E. (2020). Türkçe konuşan çocuklarda fonolojik işlemleme becerileri ilk okuma performansını yorduyor mu? Boylamsal sonuçlar. Hacettepe Üniversitesi Eğitim Fakültesi Dergisi, doi:10.16986/HUJE.2020062765
  • Felton, R. H. (1993). Effects of instruction on the decoding skills of children with phonological-processing problems. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 26(9), 583-589.
  • Felton, R. H. & Brown, I. S. (1990). Phonological processes as predictors of specific reading skills in children at risk for reading failure. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2, 212–229.
  • Furnes, B. & Samuelsson, S. (2011). Phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming predicting early development in reading and spelling: Results from a cross-linguistic longitudinal study. Learning and Individual Differences, 21(1), 85–95. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2010.10.005.
  • Georgiou, G. K., Das, J. P. & Hayward, D. V. (2008). Comparing the contribution of two tests of working memory to reading in relation to phonological awareness and rapid naming speed. Journal of Research in Reading, 31(3), 302-318.
  • Georgiou, G. K., Papadopoulos, T. C., Fella, A. & Parrila, R. (2012). Rapid naming speed components and reading development in a consistent orthography. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112, 1–17.
  • Georgiou, G. K., Parrila, R. & Papadopoulos, T. C. (2008). Predictors of word decoding and reading fluency across languages varying in orthographic consistency. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(3), 566-580.
  • Georgiou, G. K., Parrila, R., Cui, Y. & Papadopoulos, T. C. (2013). Why is rapid automatized naming related to reading? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115(1), 218-225.
  • Georgiou, G., Parrila, R., Kirby, R. & Stephenson, K. (2008). Rapid naming components and their relationship with phonological awareness, orthographic knowledge, speed of processing, and different reading outcomes. Scientific Studies of Reading, 12, 325–350.
  • Güzel Özmen, R. (2008). Öğrenme güçlüğü olan öğrenciler. In İ. H. Diken (Ed.), Özel gereksinimi olan öğrenciler ve özel eğitim (pp. 333-367). Ankara, Turkey: Pegem Akademi. doi:10.14527/9786055885267
  • Hammill, D. D. (2004). What we know about correlates of reading. Exceptional children, 70(4), 453-469. doi:10.1177/001440290407000405
  • Heikkila, R., Narhi, V., Aro, M. & Ahonen, T. (2009) Rapid automatized naming and learning disabilities: Does RAN have a specific connection to reading or not? Child Neuropsychology, 15(4), 343-358, doi:10.1080/09297040802537653
  • Karadağ, Ö., Keskin, H. K. & Arı, G. (2019). Mediatory effect of reading skill in the rapid automatized naming/reading comprehension relationship. Education and Science, 44(197), 353-366.
  • Katz, L. J., Brown, F. C., Roth, R. M. &Beers, S. R. (2011). Processing speed and working memory performance in those with both ADHD and a reading disorder compared with those with ADHD alone. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 26, 425–433.
  • Kirby, J. R., Georgiou, K., Martinussen, R. & Parrila, R. (2010). Naming speed and reading: From prediction to instruction. Reading Research Quarterly, 45, 341–362.
  • Kirby, J. R., Parrila, R. & Pfeiffer, S. (2003). Naming speed and phonological awareness as predictors of reading development. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 453–464.
  • Klein, R. M. (2002). Observations on the temporal correlates of reading failure. Reading and Writing, 15(1), 207–231.
  • Koponen, T., Aunola, K., Ahonen, T. & Nurmi, J. E. (2007). Cognitive predictors of single-digit and procedural calculation and their covariation with reading skill. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 97, 220–241.
  • Koponen, T., Salmi, P., Eklund, K. & Aro, T. (2013). Counting and RAN: Predictors of arithmetic calculation and reading fluency. Journal of Educational psychology, 105, 162- 175.
  • Korhonen, T. T. (1995). The persistence of rapid naming problems in children with reading disabilities: A nine-year follow-up. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 28, 232–239.
  • Landerl, K. & Wimmer, H. (2008). Development of word reading fluency and spelling in a consistent orthography: An 8-year follow-up. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 150–161.
  • Landerl, K., Fussenegger, B., Moll, K. & Willburger, E. (2009). Dyslexia and dyscalculia: Two learning disorders with different cognitive profiles. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103, 309–324.
  • Lovett, M. W. (1987). A developmental approach to reading disability: Accuracy and speed criteria of normal and deficient reading skill. Child Development, 58, 234–260.
  • Manis, F. R., Doi, L. M. & Bhadha, B. (2000). Naming speed, phonological awareness, and ortographic knowledge in second graders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33(4), 325–333.
  • Martinussen, R., Grimbos, T. & Ferrari, J. L. (2014). Word-level reading achievement and behavioral inattention: exploring their overlap and relations with naming speed and phonemic awareness in a community sample of children. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 29, 680–690.
  • Melby-Lervag, M., Lyster, S. A. H. & Hulme, C. (2012). Phonological skills and their role in learning to read: a meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 138(2), 322-352. Melekoğlu, M. A., Erden, G. H., & Çakıroğlu, O. (2019). Development of the Oral Reading Skills and Comprehension Test-II (SOBAT®-II) for assessment of Turkish children with specific learning disabilities: Pilot study results. Journal of Educational Issues, 5(2), 135-149 doi:10.5296/jei.v5i2.15379
  • Meyer, M. S., Wood, F. B., Hart, L. A. & Felton, R. H. (1998). Selective predictive value of rapid automatized naming in poor readers. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 31(2), 106–117.
  • NELP. (2008). Developing early literacy: Report of the National Early Literacy Panel.Washington, DC: National Institute for Literacy.
  • Neuhaus, G., Foorman, B. R., Francis, D. J. & Carlson, C. D. (2001). Measures of information processing in rapid automatizednaming (RAN) and their relation to reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 78, 359–373.
  • Norton, E. S. & Wolf, M. (2012). Rapid automatized naming (RAN) and reading fluency: Implications for understanding and treatment of reading disabilities. The Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 427–52. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100431
  • O’Malley, K. J., Francis, D. J., Foorman, B. R., Fletcher, J. M. & Swank, P. R. (2002). Growth in precursor and reading-related skills: Do low-achieving and IQ-discrepant readers develop differently? Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 17, 19–34.
  • Özata, H. & Haznedar, B. (2018). İlköğretim ikinci sınıfta akıcı sözcük okuma ve okuduğunu anlamayı etkileyen faktörler. Boğaziçi Üniversitesi Eğitim Dergisi, 35(2), 1-33.
  • Pennington, B. F., Cardoso-Martins, C., Green, P. A. & Lefly, D. L. (2001). Comparing the phonological and the double deficit hypotheses of developmental dyslexia. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 14, 707–755.
  • Poulsen, M., Juul, H. & Elbro, C. (2015). Multiple mediation analysis of the relationship between rapid naming and reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 38(2), 124-140. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9817.2012.01547.x
  • Powell, D., Stainthorp, R., Stuart, M., Garwood, H. & Quinlan, P. (2007). An experimental comparison between rival theories of rapid automatized naming performance and its relationship to reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 98(1), 46-68.
  • Puolakanaho, A., Ahonen, T., Aro, M., Eklund, K., Leppanen, P., Poikkeus, A. M., et al. (2007). Very early phonological and language skills: Estimating individual risk of reading disability. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 923–931
  • Raschle, N. M., Chang, M. & Gaab, N. (2011). Structural brain alterations associated with dyslexia predate reading onset. NeuroImage 57, 742–749.
  • Reiter, B. A. (2001). Reading disabilities related to word recognition: Underlying deficits and diagnostic approaches. Canadian Journal of School Psychology, 17, 65–84.
  • Schatschneider, C., Carlson, C. D., Francis, D. J., Foorman, B. R. & Fletcher, J. M. (2002). Relationship of rapid automatized naming and phonological awareness in early reading development: Implications for the double deficit hypothesis. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 245–256.
  • Schatschneider, C., Fletcher, J. M., Francis, D. J., Carlson, C. D. & Foorman, B. R. (2004). Kindergarten prediction of reading skills: A longitudinal comparative analysis. Journal of Education & Psychology, 96, 265–282.
  • Seçkin-Yılmaz, Ş. & Büyükçakmak, Y. (2020). Öğrenme güçlüğü olan ve olmayan öğrencilerin sözcük okuma ve ses bilgisel işlemleme becerilerinin incelenmesi. Kalem Eğitim ve İnsan Bilimleri Dergisi, 10(2), 365-391. doi:10.23863/kalem.2020.161
  • Shaul, S. & Nevo, E. (2015). Different speed of processing levels in childhood and their contribution to early literacy and reading abilities. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 32, 193-203.
  • Suk-Han Ho, C. & Ngar-Chilai, D. (1999). Naming-speed deficits and phonological memory deficits in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Learning and Individual Differences, 11(2), 173-186.
  • Temple, C. M. & Sherwood, S. (2002). Representation and retrieval of arithmetic facts: Developmental difficulties. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55, 733–752.
  • Torgesen, J. K. (2004). Avoiding the devastating downward spiral: The evidence that early intervention prevents reading failure. American Educator, 28, 6–19.
  • Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K. & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). Longitudinal studies of phonological processing and reading. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 27(5), 276-286.
  • Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C. A., Burgess, S. & Hecht, S. (1997). Contributions of phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming ability to the growth of word-reading skills in second- to fifth-grade children. Scientific Study of Reading, 1, 161–195.
  • Torppa, M., Lyytinen, P., Erskine, J., Eklund, K. & Lyytinen, H. (2010). Language development, literacy skills, and predictive connections to reading in Finnish children with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43, 308–321.
  • Vaessen, A. & Blomert, L. (2010). Long-term cognitive dynamics of fluent reading development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105, 213–231.
  • Van Bergen, E., de Jong, P. F., Maassen, B. & van der Leij, A. (2014). The effect of parents’ literacy skills and children’s preliteracy skills on the risk of dyslexia. Journal of abnormal child psychology, 42(7), 1187-1200.
  • Van der Sluis, S., De Jong, P. F. & van der Leij, A. (2004). Inhibition and shifting in children with learning deficits in arithmetic and reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87, 239–266.
  • Vellutino, F. R., Scanlon, D. M. & Tanzman, M. S. (1998). The case for early intervention in diagnosing specific reading disability. Journal of School Psychology, 36, 367–397.
  • Vukovic, R. K., Wilson, A. M. & Nash, K. K. (2004). Naming speed deficits in adults with reading disabilities Journal of Learning Disabilities, 37, 440–450.
  • Waber, D. P., Forbes, P. W., Wolff, P. H. & Weiler, M. D. (2004). Neurodevelopmental characteristics of children with learning impairments classified according to the double-deficit hypothesis. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 37, 451–461.
  • Waber, D. P., Wolff, P. H., Forbes, P. W. & Weiler, M. D. (2000). Rapid automatized naming in children referred for evaluation of heterogeneous learning problems: How specific are naming speed deficits to reading disability? Child Neuropsychology, 6, 251–261.
  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K. & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing (CTOPP). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
  • Wagner, R. K. & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101(2), 192-212.
  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K. & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). Development of reading-reiatedphonological processing abilities: New evidence of bidirectional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 30, 73-87.
  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., Hecht, S. A., Barker, T. A., Burgess, S. R. vd. (1997). Changing relations between phonological processing abilities and word-level reading as children develop from beginning to skilled readers: A 5-year longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 33(3), 468–479.
  • Walsh, D., Price, G. & Gillingham, M. (1988). The critical but transitory importance of letter naming. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 108-122.
  • Wolf, M. & Denckla, M. B. (2005). RAN/RAS: Rapid Automatized Naming and Rapid Alternating Stimulus Tests. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
  • Wolf, M. (1997). A provisional integrative account of phonological and naming speed deficits in dyslexia. In B. Blachman (Ed.), Foundation of reading acquisition (pp. 67–92). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
  • Wolf, M. & Bowers, P. (2000). The question of naming-speed deficits in developmental reading disability: An introduction to the Double-Deficit Hypothesis. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 322–324.
  • Wolf, M. & Bowers, P. G. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 415–438.
  • Wolf, M., Bally, H. & Morris, R. (1986). Automaticity, retrieval processes, and reading: A longitudinal study in average and impaired readers. Child Development, 57, 988-1005.
  • Wolf, M., Bowers, P. & Biddle, K. (2000). Naming-speed processes, timing, and reading: A conceptual review. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 387–407.
  • Wolff, U. (2014). RAN as a predictor of reading skills, and vice versa: Results from a randomised reading intervention. Annals of Dyslexia, 64, 151–165. doi:10.1007/s11881-014-0091-6
There are 90 citations in total.

Details

Primary Language Turkish
Subjects Studies on Education
Journal Section Articles
Authors

Cevriye Ergül 0000-0001-6793-6469

Ergül Demir 0000-0002-3708-8013

Project Number 215K027
Publication Date January 26, 2022
Published in Issue Year 2022 Volume: 12 Issue: 1

Cite

APA Ergül, C., & Demir, E. (2022). ANASINIFINDAN DÖRDÜNCÜ SINIFA KADAR OLAN ÇOCUKLAR İÇİN GELİŞTİRİLMİŞ HIZLI İSİMLENDİRME TESTİ’NİN GEÇERLİK VE GÜVENİRLİĞİ. Trakya Eğitim Dergisi, 12(1), 176-192. https://doi.org/10.24315/tred.883688